Investigation Mauricio Véliz



Article today
TITLE: AN EXAMINATION OF THE POSSIBLE BENEFITS OF TWO BRAIN-COMPATIBLE STRATEGIES FOR ENGLISH LANGUAGE LEARNING IN CHANCO, CHILE*
Jorge Correa Rodríguez **
Mauricio Véliz Campos ***




* Autor: Chileno, Magíster en Enseñanza de Inglés como Lengua Extranjera. Universidad Andrés Bello, Concepción-Chile. jorgecorrear@hotmail.com
***Coautor: Chileno, Dr. TESOL and Education. Universidad Católica Silva Henríquez y UNAB.
mveliz@ucsh.cl

Introduction
Since prehistoric times, human beings have felt the need to confide with their peers, looking mainly for security, support and emotional attachment, motivated for just one reason, to survive. Besides, human beings had the need to move from one place to another to escape from dangers threatening their survival. It is therefore possible to think that physical movement has been a significant part of our history and evolution. Movement keeps us alert.Let’s take the above into practice and into our teaching reality: the classroom. In some classes, for various internal, external, environmental and student-related factors, only some of these natural survival instincts mentioned above are implemented and encouraged in the learning process. Conversely, it is common to observe students listening passively to the teacher, seated for over an hour, unable to move and forced to pay attention for more than 20 minutes.

After reflecting upon these factors that affect or facilitate learning in the classroom, it would be a good self-analysis exercise of our teaching practices to be able to imagine one-moment learning as a real ‘threat’ to the survival of our students. Thus, it would not be strange to think that the instinctive search for help and protection in others to survive would be the only natural and biologically accepted way in our brain to cope with this threat. It is for this reason that we as teachers should reflect on the real and positive effectiveness of peer learning, inclusion of physical movement and the role of attention span on the basis of a survival-based assumption.


Context of the study
This research aims to examine the possible benefits that brain-based compatible strategies have in two groups of students from a public High School in Chanco, a small seaside town in the Maule region of Chile, when learning English as a foreign language.


Brain-compatible Learning
In order to illustrate that brain-compatible learning is not necessarily a new trend or a set of strategies, a brief historical description is presented as follows.

In 1862, Broca found real evidence to connect language learning with specific areas of the brain, (LePointe, 2012). Years later, Wernicke was also able to find supplementary evidence linking language learning to the brain. These results showed that specific parts of the brain have specific functions linked to language learning. All these results led to the brain lateralization theory, which has been significant for the relevant field over the last 30 years.

In 1983, Hart dubbed the brain as ‘the organ of learning’. It was one of the first steps to focus learning research on the biological processes the brain engages in when learning new information, in this case learning a second language. The relevance of this statement is that the brain, as any organ, has physical processes that can be enhanced by setting the proper conditions when learning anything.

If educators teach their students, they are making changes in their students’ brains. Therefore, it would not be strange to think that educators need to be aware of the new findings neuroscience can provide in order to make learning in students more appropriate and natural for them.

According to Caine & Caine (1994), ‘Although all learning is brain based in some sense, to us brain based learning involves acknowledging the brain’s rules for meaningful learning and organizing teaching with those rules in mind. That is when we are teaching to the human brain’ (p.4).  The authors imply that educators need to know more about how the brain processes information inside the learner’s brain. If they do it, then, educators will be facilitating learning.

Jensen (2008) claims that ‘brain-based education is the engagement of strategies based on principles derived from an understanding of the brain’ (p.4). The author stresses the importance of managing brain learning processing information when planning teaching to improve learning. On the other hand, Zull (2002) advocates for ‘(…) creating conditions that lead to change in a learner’s brain. We can’t get inside and rewire a brain, but we can arrange things so that it gets rewired.’

He goes on to say that ‘if we are skilled, we can set up conditions that favor this rewiring, and we can create an environment that nurtures it’ (p.5). Thus, the aim of this research is to examine two variables that might influence learning, to promote a more natural way of acquiring information. These two definitions can be summarized as follows: When learning, teachers need to promote and use strategies in the classrooms by setting the right and natural conditions the brain needs for learning.


Methodological framework              
In order to establish the possible effects that the inclusion of physical movement and attention-span have when learning English, a quasi-experimental design was selected. This methodology is understood as the research method which lacks the random assignment of the participants of the study compared to the experimental method.

According to Cohen and Manion (2007), ‘the field or quasi-experiment in the natural setting rather than the laboratory, but where variables are isolated, controlled and manipulated’ (p. 274). In this case, an English classroom is the setting in which this research is conducted.

Thus, this quasi-experimental study was designed as pre and post-test control group model. In order to maintain a reasonable level of objectivity the experimental and control groups were selected keeping in mind that they need to share similar characteristics in relation to age, context, level of English and gender.
One group of students is defined as the experimental group and the other as the control group. Both classes are tested using pre and post-tests before and after the experiment.

A quantitative approach is used as a way of gathering the information. It is considered more appropriate because of the experimental nature of this research. The researcher will compare two similar groups using an independent variable to test its impact on a dependent variable.



The treatment
As explained earlier, two similar groups of students are part of this study. During the research process, the experimental group is treated with a brain-compatible learning approach, while the control group is given a traditional teacher-centered teaching approach with a great deal of teacher talking time. In order to establish whether the use of physical movement and the role of attention-span theory a pre and post-test format was administered.

The pre-test was applied before the intervention began, after which came the treatment proper, which spanned two weeks after which a post-test, containing exactly the same tests items, was applied.

The aim of this research is to compare the results from the post-test, which includes the contents covered in the classes learners received during the time the experiment was conducted. The control group is taught using to a traditional teaching approach, mainly teacher-led.

The experimental group received a brain-compatible approach of learning, in which micro-classes of 15 minutes are presented to the students; the same content in both groups was selected to be taught in order to isolate the variables as much as possible in the experiment.

Apart from the micro-classes, learners are instructed through the use of gestures or mimicry, including physical movement within the class.  Movement was included through the implementation of some ‘power teaching’ strategies. These strategies were taught during the first semester to the students.

Micro classes have a five minute break to re-start the students’ attention-span. The idea is to facilitate the brain’s processes that promote the acquisition and consolidation of knowledge acquisition.

 The time allocated to present the unit is eight hours. Each group has four hours of 90 minutes a week, divided in two hours a day. The specific procedure implemented is described below.


Limitations
This research encountered some limitations that are briefly outlined below:

Time: This research was carried out during the second semester, specifically in November, which is why the researcher had to apply the treatment only in two weeks as the academic year was almost over, which may have had an impact on students’ level of engagement.

This is the first year the main researcher taught the students; he met them only the first semester of this year as students came from two different primary schools in Chanco. In such primary schools students did not have specialized EFL teachers.

The research was halted for one week during the experiment due to various extra-curricular activities the school had and the presidential elections. During that week only the experimental group received two hours of treatment.


Results
Pre-tests Results
The pre-test was applied before the intervention was performed in both groups. The main objective was to identify the actual level of knowledge students had in relation to the teaching point taught before both brain-compatible strategies were implemented in the experimental group.
The results in both groups showed that most of the students were not able to identify past continuous structures, namely how to use operators was and were with the corresponding pronoun or subject, the (-ing) ending in verbs, how to transform sentences into negative, interrogative or affirmative forms.


Post-tests Results
The results of the post-test are presented in the same way the pre-test. The post-test was applied after the intervention was performed in both groups.

The main objective was to identify how much learning had taken place throughout the unit after brain-compatible strategies were implemented in the experimental group.

The results in the experimental group showed that most of the students were able to identify past continuous structures, namely how to use the operators was and were with the corresponding pronoun, the ing ending in verbs; how to transform sentences into negative or affirmative in the above mentioned tense.
In the reading section most of the students were able to answer the questions; as well, in the listening part, most of them answered correctly. In the control group most of them were not able to recognize these structures and had difficulty dealing with the skills, specifically listening.

The results showed that most of the students obtained a score over 4.0 in the experimental group. These results are presented below, wherein a comparison between both groups is made.

It is important to mention that three students were absent during the application of the post-test: Two students from the experimental group and one student from the control group.


Do brain-compatible learning strategies improve the learning of a foreign language?
After analyzing the results in both groups it can be observed that in the experimental group most of the students obtained a score over a 4.0 (equivalent to the pass mark in Chile, on a scale of 1.0 to 7.0), while in the control group fewer students obtained a score over a 4.0. 

As shown earlier in Table 9, the results illustrate that it is possible to think that brain compatible learning strategies improve the learning of a foreign language.

At this point, it would be reasonable to explain that the researcher is aware that these two brain-compatible learning procedures produce an observable effect on the experimental group but it is unknown which of the two variables specifically improved learning more significantly, or whether it was a mixture of them that enhanced learning in students from the experimental group.


Does the inclusion of physical movement improve the learning of a foreign language?
After analyzing the results in the post-tests in both groups, it is possible to observe that there is an evident improvement in the scores in the experimental group. It is therefore reasonable to argue that the inclusion of physical movement to improve the learning of a foreign language does occur.

On the other hand, in the control group, in which the students did not have the presence of this variable, fewer students got high scores.

Graph 7 shows the results in which it can be observed that the inclusion of physical movement improves the learning of a foreign language. On the contrary, in the control group the mean scores were lower; the absence of this variable in that group might have led to those results.


Does attention-span relate to learning efficiency in first year secondary students learning a foreign language?
The relationship between the attention-span and learning efficiency can be observed in the experimental group wherein the mean scores were higher and the standard deviation was lower, in stark contrast with the control group. It can be inferred that there is a connection between this specific variable and the learning of a foreign language. Once again, it is unknown if the specific impact of this variable really affected the learning of a foreign language or if it was just the mixture of both variables.


Conclusions
At the end of the study and after analyzing the results the researchers are able to observe that the results showed some interesting findings. It is possible to think that there is a positive and effective relationship between the two variables selected for this study: The inclusion of physical movement and the role of attention-span when learning a foreign language in high schools students.

It would be also advisable to conduct new research using other brain compatible strategies such as sleep effects, foods, emotions, social brain, to name some.

This research is a quantitative study; nevertheless it was observed that students from the experimental group showed higher levels of commitment and participation in classes, qualitative features that are not accounted for in the present investigation. For further research, a qualitative intervention should be advisable to be included in order to identify student’s impression and feelings towards the implementation of these brain-compatible strategies in the experimental group and also to get information about student’s perceptions within the control group.


REFERENCES
Biffle, C. (2010). Whole brain teaching for challenging kids (and the rest of your class, too!). Yucaipa, California: Crafton Hills College.

Caine, G., & Caine, R. N. (1994). Making Connections. Teaching and the human brain.
MenloPark, California: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.

Caine, R. N., Caine, G., McClintic, C., &Klimek, K. J. (2009). 12 brain/mind learning principles in action: Developing executive functions of the human brain (2nd Ed.). Thousand Oaks, Cl: Corwin Press.

Cohen, L, Manion, L & Morrison, K. (2007). Research Methods in Education, 6thEdition, London: Routledge.
Erlauer, L. (2003). The brain-compatible classroom: Using what we know about    learning to improve teaching. Virginia: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Hannaford, C. (2005). Smart moves. Arlington, VA: Great River Books.

Hart, Leslie. (1983). Human brain and human learning. Longman Publishing Group.

Jensen, E. (1998). Teaching with the brain and mind. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.
Jensen, E. (2008). Brain-based learning: The new paradigm of teaching. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
LePointe, L. (2012). Paul Broca and the origins of language in the brain. San Diego, Plural Publishing, Inc.

Medina, J. (2008). Brain rules: 12 Principles for surviving and thriving at work, home and school. Seattle: Pear Press.

Ramon y Cajal, S. (1929). Studies on vertebrate neurogenesis. Trans. L. GUTH. 1929. Reprint. Springfield, Ill.: Thomas.

Sousa, D. A. (2011). How the brain learns. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.

Schmidt, Richard W. (1995). Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In Richard W. Schmidt (ed.), Attention and Awareness in Foreign Language Learning, 1-63.  Honolulu, Ha: University of Hawai’i Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center.

Tokuhama-Espinosa, T. (2011). Mind, brain, and education science: A comprehensive guide to the new brain-based learning. New York, NY:   W. W. Norton.

Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasischesymptomencomplex. Breslau: Kohn and Weigert

Willis, J., (2007). Brain-friendly strategies for the inclusion classroom. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.

Zull, J.E. (2002). The art of changing the brain: Enriching the practice of teaching by Exploring the Biology of Learning. Sterling Virginia: Stylus Publishing.